Insects are traditionally thought to respond to noxious stimuli in an inflexible manner, without the ability to modulate their behavior according to context. In new research, scientists from the United Kingdom and Poland investigated whether bumblebees’ attraction to high sucrose solution concentrations reduces their avoidance of noxious (painful) heat. Their results, published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, show that the bees can modify their response to noxious stimuli in a manner that is viewed in other animals as consistent with the ability to feel pain.
Gibbons et al. showed that bumblebees are capable of modifying their response to noxious stimuli in order to get a higher sugar reward. Image credit: Ralphs Fotos.
“Insects used to be regarded as simple reflex automatons, responding to damaging stimuli only by withdrawal reflexes,” said study’s senior author Professor Lars Chittka, a researcher in the School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences at Queen Mary University of London.
“Our new work shows that bees’ responses are more flexible and that they can suppress such reflexes when it suits them, for example if there is an extra-sweet treat to be had.”
“Such flexibility is consistent with the capacity of a subjective experience of pain.”
“Scientists traditionally viewed insects as unfeeling robots, which avoid injury with simple reflexes,” said study’s first author Matilda Gibbons, a Ph.D. student in the School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences at Queen Mary University of London.
“We’ve discovered bumblebees respond to harm non-reflexively, in ways that suggest they feel pain.”
“If insects can feel pain, humans have an ethical obligation not to cause them unnecessary suffering.”
“But the UK’s animal welfare laws don’t protect insects — our study shows that perhaps they should.”
In their study, the authors used a motivational trade-off paradigm, where animals must flexibly trade-off two competing motivations.
Buff-tailed bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) were given the choice between either unheated or noxiously-heated (55 degrees Celsius) feeders with different sucrose concentrations and marked by different colors.
When both feeders were high-quality and one of them was noxiously heated, bees tended to avoid the heated feeder.
But the bees were more likely to use the heated feeders when they contained a higher concentration sucrose.
The researchers also expanded on the motivational trade-off paradigm by ensuring that the trade-off relied on cues (colors) that the bees had learned to associate with a higher sugar reward.
Because the bees used learnt color cues for their decisions, the trade-off was based on processing in the brain, rather than just peripheral processing.
In other words, the bees decided to undergo some pain or discomfort in order to get a higher sugar reward.
This showed that the trade-off is mediated in the central nervous system — an ability that is viewed in other animals as consistent with the capacity to feel pain.
Because of the subjective nature of pain experience, this not a formal proof, but the possibility of insect pain and suffering should be taken seriously.
“Insects (unlike vertebrates) are not currently protected by any legislation regarding their treatment in research laboratories and in the growing industry that produces insects for human consumption or as food for conventional livestock,” Professor Chittka said.
“The legal framework for the ethical treatment of animals may have to be expanded.”
“The increasing evidence for some form of sentience in insects places on us an obligation to conserve the environments that have shaped their unique and seemingly alien minds.”
“We humans are only one of many species capable of enjoyment and suffering, including pain-like states.”
“Even miniature creatures such as insects deserve our respect and ethical treatment and a duty to minimize suffering where it is in our power to do so.”
_____
Matilda Gibbons et al. 2022. Motivational trade-offs and modulation of nociception in bumblebees. PNAS 119 (31): e2205821119; doi: 10.1073/pnas.2205821119
Source link: https://www.sci.news/biology/bumblebee-pain-11060.html